Monday 13 October 2014

Real World Altruism

Introductory waffle


This is very much a work-in-progress blog post, written in a bit of a rush. I hope to edit it ‘sometime’ to make it more clear, readable, and engaging, especially for non psychology students.

During the second seminar of my altruism module, each student chooses a behaviour that might sometimes be, at least in part, motivated by altruism, i.e., concern for the positive welfare of another or others. They can but do not have to choose from the following list:

adoption, blood donation, body-part (e.g., bone marrow, egg, sperm, organ) donation, business practices, caring, citizenship, courageous resistance (including whistle-blowing), courtesy, driving, environmentalism and animal welfare, heroism and emergency intervention, honesty, internet behaviour, liberalism, lying, mercy killing, philanthropy (charitable giving), religious action, rescuing, romantic relationships, sexual behaviour, shopping, social activism, surrogacy, tax-paying, terrorism, tipping, vaccination, volunteering, working, etc.

Each student then spends part of the following week researching links between altruism (as defined above) and their chosen behaviour. They produce a poster summarising what they have found important or interesting. In the third seminar, they explain their posters to each other and explore implications of similarities and differences across behaviours. We end with a whole-group discussion. The vast majority of students put a lot of thought and effort into their posters, which do not contribute to the grade they will get for the module. Thanks, guys. You rock.

Below are some of the more important issues that tend to come out during whole group discussions of students’ findings – albeit sometimes with a heavy steer from me.


Behavioural specificity


If one’s goal is to identify and understand causes of something, it is crucially important to be very precise about what that thing is. Otherwise explaining, identifying, measuring, and manipulating that thing will at best be a hit-and-miss affair. At worst, the misses will far outnumber the hits.

I have spent a considerable amount of time detailing precisely how I think the term “altruism” can usefully be understood (e.g., here, here, here and especially here). It is important to also be as clear as possible about any behaviour alleged to stem from or express such altruism.

“Organ donation”, for example, is not the same thing as “giving consent for one’s organs to be posthumously removed and used for transplant or research purposes”. It is common for people to be enthusiastic about the former general concept but disconcerted when thinking about the latter specific possibility. As recognised long ago (Farsides, 2000) and recently demonstrated empirically (Seiegal et al., 2014), attitudes towards these distinct things can differ considerably.

This is not pure semantics. Champions of behavioural markers of altruism rarely give due attention to what, specifically, they mean by the target behaviour. This is particularly true if common terminology sneaks in words that might encourage assumptions of an altruistic motive, e.g., “caring”, “donating”, “giving”, “volunteering”, etc.. It is perfectly possible to “give” someone a lift in a car in return for money - in which case, of course, it is not a gift.

“Sure, I can give you a lift”

Which of the following is the behaviour that best represents a specific act of “blood donation”: allowing someone to take blood out of one’s arm; allowing someone to take a blood sample to be tested for a medical condition; allowing someone to take blood from one’s arm in return for pay; allowing a vampire to feed on one’s blood in exchange for a promise of immortality; or allowing someone to take blood from one’s arm because one has an expectation that it will be used to try to improve someone else’s welfare? If one chooses the first option it encompasses all the others and there will be no distinction between people ‘giving’ blood for personal gain and people giving blood in an attempt to help others (or indeed for any other reason). If one wishes to restrict the phrase “personal act of organ donation” to something like the final option (as I would), one no longer has a strictly behavioural indicator. Instead, one needs to have a pretty good idea that people are giving or donating their blood in an attempt to improve others’ welfare. One needs to know something about their goals and motives.

Monroe et al. (1990) and London (1970) reached very different conclusions about the motives of people who engaged in the behaviour of “rescuing Jews from Nazi persecution in wartime Europe”. This may be because London allowed in his sample people who received pay for their rescue and Monroe et al. omitted such people. “Rescuing”, “rescuing for pay”, and “rescuing without pay” are arguably very different behaviours, quite possibly with distinct antecedents, explanations, and consequences.

Ditto “volunteering”, “mandatory volunteering”, and voluntary volunteering (Stukas et al., 1999).

Now you try! What is an act of “giving to charity?” Is it transferring ownership of money or other resources from one person to a charity (for whatever reason, e.g., tax-avoidance); is it a person doing this specifically in an attempt to help the charity in some way (e.g., as a vanity project); or is it a person doing this in an attempt to help whoever or whatever the charity claims to act in support of?  These are not identical things! Only the first is purely a behaviour. The others are behaviours engaged in for particular reasons.


Focused altruism


When they are altruistic, people want good things for specific others. Their attitude towards people and things other than the targets of altruism (including their selves) cannot be taken for granted: it could be similarly positively concerned, indifferent, or indeed aggressive. Someone who wants a particular charity to thrive may not care about the fate of other charities. Someone who wants a particular country to thrive may want other countries to suffer in pursuit of this goal. People who want the best possible life for their children may have no active concern about any other child.


Multiple motives


People can have different motives for doing the same behaviour. Any behavior that is sometimes motivated at least in part by concern for the positive welfare of others is sometimes also or instead motivated by non-altruistic concerns. I cannot think of a single exception to this claim.

This is true for any all ‘prosocial’ or ‘helping’ behaviours, including caring (Cicirelli, 1993; Feeney & Collins, 2003); celebrity endorsement (Kelly et al., 2014); community involvement (Batson et al., 2002); egg donation (Kenney & McGowan, 2008; Limiñana-Gras et al., 2014); environmentalism (Gagnon-Thompson & Barton, 1994 ; Schultz et al., 2005); giving blood (Sojka & Sojka, 2008); ethical purchasing and investment (Bravo et al., 2012; Nilsson, 2009); organ donation (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003; Guedj et al., 2011; Robbins et al., 2001; Sanner, 2006); philanthropy (Piferi et al., 2006; Schervish, 2005); rescuing (Reicher et al., 2006); safer sex (Nimmons & Folkman, 1999); social activism (Cable et al, 1988; Russell, 2011); surrogacy (Blyth, 1994; van Zyl & Walker, 2012); and volunteering (Allison et al., 2002; Clary et al., 1998; Omoto & Snyder, 1995).

It is also true that practices often engaged in for self-interested reasons can also or instead be pursued for altruistic ones, for example paid work (Amabile et al., 1994; Brewer et al., 2002; Coursey & Pandey, 2007; Wrzesniewski et al., 1997), vaccination (Hershey et al., 1994), staying alive (Russell, 1999), and suicide (Blake, 1978; Van Orden et al., 2010).

Similarly, behaviours many think of as immoral, antisocial, or otherwise inappropriate can also be motivated, at least sometimes and in part, by concern for the positive welfare of others, e.g., cheating (Gino et al., 2013); civil disobedience (Passini & Morselli, 2011); lying (DePaulo & Kashy, 1998); killing and letting die (Freidman & Resnick, 2007; Georges et al., 2007); military disobedience (Linn, 1996); terrorism (Ginges et al., 2011; Kruglanski & Fishman, 2009; Victoroff, 2005); and whistle-blowing (Kesselheim et al., 2010).

Finally, sometimes people refrain from particular behaviours because they think that any attempted help would not actually be in the best interests of those whose welfare they care about (Gilbert & Silvera, 1996).

A consequence of all this is that behavioural markers of altruism are a lot more difficult to identify than might be thought. Many behaviours that are often motivated by concern for the positive welfare of others are sometimes motivated by narrow self-interest (or worse) and many behaviours (including “doing nothing”) that are often motivated by narrow self-interest (or worse) are sometimes motivated, at least in part, by concern for the positive welfare of others.

To be scientific and to have a justified claim to inform policy and practice, theories of altruism need to explain and be informed by people’s behaviour. But a science of altruism is unlikely to produce good theories unless careful attention is paid to the relationship between particular behaviours and the role played in their production by concerns for the welfare of others (and other motives). Many theories considered on this module make bold claims to be scientific because they focus on behaviours and not the motives that may lead to them. Such an approach may (or may not) provide good accounts of the origins of those behaviours but they (a) need to be very clear about the defining characteristics of those behaviours, and (b) should not assume that those theories tell us tell us very much about the phenomenon I am most interested in, i.e., concern for the positive welfare of others. This is even true - indeed it is especially true - if they claim to be studying “altruistic” behaviours.


“X can cause Y” does not entail that “X (only and always) causes Y”


That “X can cause Y” does not mean either that “X will always cause Y” or that “X is the only cause of Y”. Milgram (1970) demonstrated that obedience to authority can cause people to administer apparently fatal electric shocks to someone pleading for their torture to stop. This does not mean that obedience to authority always leads to torture (e.g., an authority may forbid torture) and nor does it mean that people only torture others because of orders to do so (e.g., they may take it upon themselves to engage in torture whether authorities encourage or dissuade such behaviour).

I honestly find it difficult to believe how many ‘scientific’ papers demonstrate that a particular motive can lead to an allegedly altruistic behaviour and then invite or reach the conclusion that the former “therefore” (always) leads to the latter and that the latter “therefore” is always the result of the former. This drives me nuts!

Let me give you an example; one of many.

Griskevicius et al. (2010) demonstrated that people’s concerns for status can motivate them to pay high prices for ‘green’ products when doing so is a route to the status they desire. I have no issue with this other than thinking, “Well, duh!” I do have an issue with a claim that such a finding provides good evidence either that people with status concerns will always pay high prices for ‘green’ products or, more importantly, that all behaviour apparently motivated by environmental or altruistic concerns is in fact always and only motivated by status concerns. To me, such a conclusion is beyond silly. Readers doubtful about this are invited to re-read the section immediately above.


Changing Motives


Reasons for initially engaging in particular behaviours may differ from reasons for continuing to engage in those behaviours. People may start doing something for one reason but keep doing it for other reasons. People may also start doing something for one reason and then stop doing it for exactly the same reason, e.g., if someone volunteers to cheer themselves up but then discovers that volunteering makes them more miserable.

Accordingly, people may start doing something for altruistic reasons but continue for non-altruistic reasons (Nelson & Folbre, 2006) or vice versa (Callero et al., 1987; Charng et al., 1988; Ferguson et al., 2012). Similarly, people may start something for one set of reasons but stop for the same or for different reasons (Beldad et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2003; Schroer & Hertel, 2009; Snyder et al., 1999).

In other words, just as motives can differ across people (see “Multiple motives” above), they can also change (‘within’ people) across time.


Situational motives


Motives can also differ across situations. Concern for the positive welfare of others depends on all manner of things that vary across situations, e.g., the others’ perceived need, perceived opportunities for change, how much the others are considered deserving of good or bad things happening to them, etc. We will study a whole raft of such factors later in the module.


Mixed Motives


People often have a variety of motives that exist at the same time or in quick succession. Motives can act in concert to promote particular behaviours or they can be incompatible and result in confusion or apparent inconsistency (Campbell, 2010; Merrell, 2000).

The considerations in the last few sections reiterate the main point made at the end of the first: there is no one-to-one correspondence between a particular behaviour (e.g., giving money to someone else) and a particular concern motivating that behaviour (e.g., altruism, narrow self-interest, morality, aggression...). The same motive (or set of motives) can lead to different behaviours and the same behaviour can stem from different motives (or different sets of motives). As well as being important when trying to predict and explain particular behaviours, such issues will be important again next week when we consider measures of altruism.


It’s not just self vs. other


Because of the history of the word “altruism” and on-going theoretical confusion, it is common for people to consider altruism in terms of a conflict between self- and other- interest. It is important to realise that there are far more interests at stake than this.

Because people can be concerned about the positive welfare of various others, altruistic concerns can co-exist and sometimes conflict with each other and with other, non-altruistic, concerns. People can be simultaneously concerned about “the general good”, the welfare of various specific others, their own welfare, doing what they consider to be God’s will, etc. (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2010; Kurtz et al., 2005). 


“If I had to choose between betraying my country and betraying my friend I hope I should have the guts to betray my country”. E. M. Forster


Multiple methods


People can pursue all manner of different methods to seek to establish particular aspects of others’ positive welfare (Monnickendam et al., 2007; Van der Cingel, 2011). Someone who “wants to make the world a better place” may attempt to do so by giving blood, being political active, volunteering, or doing any number of other things. This again reiterates the point that there is not a one-to-one correspondence between altruism and any particular behaviour.


Altruism matters


It is common to hear people say things like, “As long as the behaviour keeps happening, who cares what motivates it or if such motives are altruistic or not?” I do and I think you should.

If people help others only instrumentally, in service of, for example, narrow self-interest, they are likely to provide such help only to the extent that it keeps serving that particular interest. If those motives are better served by not helping, then helping will stop. If the motives are adequately served by low-effort, poor-quality behaviour, then low-effort, poor quality helping is what is likely to be provided. If the motives come to be seen as better served by being unhelpful or maybe even callous or aggressive, well...

To the extent that people help others because they seek those others’ positive welfare, their helping will track what seems to be in the others’ interest and it will be less dictated and distracted by other (e.g., self-) interests.

In lots of different ways, people provide better care for others when they genuinely care about them (i.e., about their positive welfare) than when they provide ‘care’ primarily in pursuit of narrow self-interest (i.e., to obtain benefits solely for the self): Bendapudi et al. (1996); Cappellari and Turati (2004); Clary and Miller (1986); Finkelstein (2009); Horn (2012); Mowen and Sujan (2005); Penner and Finkelstein (1998); Shim et al. (2012); van Zyl and Walker (2012); Wright (2007).


Altruism can be rewarding


People concerned about the positive welfare of others can find helping them rewarding (Berg et al., 2001; Brouwer et al., 2005; Harbaugh et al., 2007). This does not mean that they are impervious to costs of helping (Snyder et al., 1999).


Enough is enough


People hope for satisfaction of their desires and seek satisfaction of their motives. People’s desires and motives fade when satisfied. People who merely want to make a donation to charity want to do that. They do not necessarily want to give to the most effective charity and they will not necessarily welcome an ‘opportunity’ to work out which of several charities is most effective (Breeze, 2010; Buchheit & Parsons, 2006).

Any reader doubtful about this might want to re-read the “Behavioural specificity” section above.


Barriers can be as important as motives


Encouraging altruism has two often-related but clearly distinct aspects: encouraging particular motives and removing barriers that inhibit people from having or acting on such motives.

Just as there are multiple motives for engaging in any particular behaviour, so too are there multiple barriers (Guedj et al., 2011; Hyde et al., 2012; Morgan, 2006; Newton, 2011; Sanner, 2006; Sojka & Sojka, 2008).


Context matters (a lot)


Altruism and helping are more pronounced in contexts where such qualities are valued and rewarded than they are in other contexts and they are least pronounced in contexts where those qualities are anathema and punished (Abolafia, 2002; Lois, 2007; Park & Smith, 2007; Rotolo & Wilson, 2006).


 Ability and opportunity matters


Effective helping requires means and opportunities as well as motives (Bekkers, 2005; Gillath et al., 2005; Gross, 1994; Stern et al., 1999).

To the extent that altruistic helping requires specific abilities and opportunities, distinct sets of qualities will need to accompany altruism for such helping to be effectively provided (Rand & Epstein, 2014; Shepela et al., 1999).

  

References


Abolafia, M. Y. (2002). Making markets: Opportunism and restraint on Wall Street. In N. W. Biggart (Ed.), Economic Sociology (Ch 6, pp. 94-111). Blackwell.
Allison, L.D; Okun, M.A and Dutridge. K.S. (2002). Assessing volunteer motives: a comparison of an open ended probe and Likert rating scales.  Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 243-255. 
Amabile, T.M., Hill, K.., Hennessey, B, A., & Tighe, E.M. (1994). The Work Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 950-967.
Batson,C.D., Ahmad,N., & Tsang, J-A. (2002). Four motives for community involvement. Journal of Social Issues, 3, 429-446.
Bekkers, R. (2005). Participation in voluntary associations: Relations with resources, personality, and political values. Political Psychology, 26 (3), 439-454.
Beldad, A., Snip, B., & van Hoof, J. (2012). Generosity the second time around: Determinants of individuals’ repeat donation intention. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 43(1), 144-163.
Bendapudi, N, Singh, S. N., & Bendapudi, V. (1996). Enhancing helping behaviour: an integrative framework for promotion planning. Journal of Marketing, 60, 33-49.
Berg, M. B., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Cotter, J. (2001). Perceiving value in obligations and goals: Wanting to do what should be done. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(8), 982-995. 9).
Blake, J. A. (1978). Death by hand grenade: Altruistic suicide in combat. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior8(1), 46-59.
Bleske-Rechek, A., Nelson, L. A., Baker, J. P., Remiker, M. W., & Brandt, S. J. (2010). Evolution and the trolley problem: People save five over one unless the one is young, genetically related, or a romantic partner. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology4(3), 115-127.
Blyth, E. (1994). “i wanted to be interesting. i wanted to be able to say ‘i've done something interesting with my life’”: Interviews with surrogate mothers in Britain. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology12(3), 189-198.
Bravo, C. P., Cordts, A., Schulze, B., & Spiller, A. (2012). Assessing determinants of organic food consumption using data from the German National Nutrition Survey II. Food Quality and Preference, 28 (1), 60-70.
Breeze, B. (2010). How donors choose charities. Findings of a study of donor perceptions of the nature and distribution of charitable benefit. Centre for Charitable Giving and Philanthropy (CGAP), University of Kent. Ocassional Paper 1.
Brewer, G. A., Sheldon, S. C., & Facer, R. L. II. (2002). Individual conceptions of public service motivation. Public Administration Review, 60 (3), 254-264.
Brouwer, W. B. F., van Exel, N. J. A., van der Berg, B., van den Bos, G. A. M. & Koopmanschap, M. A. (2005). Process utility from providing informal care: The benefits of caring. Health Policy, 74, 85-99.
Buchheit, S., & Parsons, L. M. (2006). An experimental investigation of accounting information’s influence on the individual giving process. Journal of Accounting and Public Policy25(6), 666-686.
Cable, S., Walsh, E. J., & Warland, R. H. (1988). Differential paths to political activism: Comparisons of four mobilization processes after the Three Mile Island accident. Social Forces, 66, 951-969.
Callero, P. L., Howard, J. A., & Piliavin, J. A. (1987). Helping behavior as role behavior: disclosing social structure and history in the analysis of prosocial action. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50, 247-256.
Campbell, B. (2010). Contradictory behaviour during genocides. Sociological Forum, 25 (2), 296-314. 
Cappellari, L., & Turati, G. (2004). Volunteer labour supply: The role of workers’ motivations. Annals of Public and Cooperative Economics, 75(4), 619-643.
Charng, H-W., Piliavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role identity and reasoned action in the prediction of repeated behaviour. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 303-317.
Cicirelli, V. G. (1993). Attachment and obligation as daughters’ motives for caregiving behavior and subsequent effect on subjective burden. Psychology and Aging, 8, 144-155. 
Clary, E. G., & Miller, J. (1986). Socialization and situational influences on sustained altruism. Child Development, 57, 1358-1369. 
Clary, E. G., Snyder, M., Ridge, R. D., Copeland, J., Stukas, A. A., Haugen, J., & Miene, P. (1998). Understanding and assessing the motivations of volunteers: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1516-1530.  
Coursey, D. H., & Pandey, S. K. (2007). Public service motivation measurement: Testing an abridged version of Perry’s proposed Scale.. Administration and Society, 39 (5), 547-568.
Davis, M. E., Hall, J. A., & Meyer, M. (2003). The first year: Influences on the satisfaction, involvement, and persistence of new community volunteers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 248-260. 
DePaulo, B.M., & Kashy, D.A. (1998). Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 63-79. 
Farsides, T. (2000). Winning hearts and minds: Using psychology to promote voluntary organ donation. Health Care Analysis, 8, 101-121. 
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2003). Motivations for caregiving in adult intimate relationships: influences on caregiving behaviour and relationship functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 950-968.
Ferguson, E., Atsma, F., de Kort, W., & Veldhuizen, I. (2012). Exploring the pattern of blood donor beliefs in first-time, novice, and experienced donors: Differentiating reluctant altruism, pure altruism, impure altruism, and warm glow. Transfusion, 52 (2), 343-355. 
Finkelstein, M. A. (2009). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivational orientations and the volunteer process. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 653-658.
Freidman, S. H., & Resnick, P. J. (2007). Child murder by mothers: Patterns and prevention. World Psychiatry, 6 (3), 137 – 141. 
Gagnon-Thompson, S. C., & Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric and anthropcentric attitudes towards the environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14, 149-157.
Georges, J. J., Onwuteaka-Philipsen, B. D., Muller, M. T., Van der Wal, G., Van der Heide, A., & Van der Maas, P. J. (2007). Relatives' perspective on the terminally ill patients who died after euthanasia or physician-assisted suicide: a retrospective cross-sectional interview study in the Netherlands. Death Studies, 31(1), 1-15.
Gilbert, D. T., & Silvera, D. H. (1996). Overhelping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 678-690.
Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Nitzberg, R. E., Erez, A., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Attachment, caregiving, and volunteering: Placing volunteerism in an attachment-theoretical framework. Personal Relationships, 12, 425-446. 
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Sachdeva, S., & Medin, D. (2011). Psychology out of the laboratory: The challenge of violent extremism. American Psychologist66(6), 507.
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely, D. (2013). Self-serving altruism? The lure of unethical actions that benefit others. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization93, 285-292.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (3), 392-404.
Gross, M. L. (1994). Jewish rescue in Holland and France during the second world war: Moral cognition and collective action. Social Forces, 73, 463-496. 
Guedj, M., Sastre, M., & Mullet, E. (2011). Donating organs: A theory-driven inventory of motives. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 16 (4), 418-429.
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316, 1622-1625.
Hershey, J. C., Asch, D. A., Thumasathit, T., Meszaros, J., & Waters, V. V. (1994). The roles of altruism, free riding, and bandwagoning in vaccination decisions. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 59, 177–187. 
Horn, A. S. (2012). The cultivation of a prosocial value orientation through community service: An examination of organizational context, social facilitation, and duration. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41 (7), 948-968.
Hyde, M. K., Wihardjo, K. R., & White, K. M. (2012). Do the myths still exist? Revisiting people’s negative beliefs about organ donation upon death. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 17(5), 530-541.
Johnson, E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science, 302, 1338-1339.
Joiner, T. E., Pettit, J. W., Walker, R. L., Voelz, Z. R., Cruz, J., Rudd, M. D., & Lester, D. (2002). Perceived burdensomeness and suicidality: Two studies on the suicide notes of those attempting and those completing suicide. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology21(5), 531-545.
Kelly, S. P., Morgan, G. G., &Coule, T. M. (2014). Celebrity altruism: The good, the bad and the ugly in relationships with fundraising charities. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing19(2), 57-75.
Kenney, N. J., & McGowan, M. L. (2008). Looking back: Egg donors’ retrospective evaluations of their motivations, expectations, and experiences during their first donation cycle. Fertility and Sterility, 93(2), 455-466.
Kesselheim, A. S., Studdert, D. M., Mello, M. M. (2010). Whistle-blowers' experiences in fraud litigation against pharmaceutical companies. New England Journal of Medicine, 362, 1832-1939.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Fishman, S. (2009). Psychological factors in terrorism and counterterrorism: individual, group and organisational levelsof analysis. Social Issues and Policy Review, 3 (1), 1-44.
Kurtz, T., Donaghue, N., Rapley, M., & Walker, I. (2005). The ways that people talk about natural resources: Discursive strategies as barriers to environmentally sustainable practices. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 603-620. 
Limiñana-Gras, R. M., Colodro-Conde, L., Andrés-Giménez, E. D., Sarabia-Cos, L., Vincenti, M. A., Giral-Sánchez, B., & Ruíz-Mira, C. M. (2014). Altruism and oocyte donation: Motivational dimensions and decisions in women donors. Personality and Individual Differences60, S50.
Linn, R. (1996). When the individual soldier says “no” to war: A look at selective refusal during the Intifada. Journal of Peace Research, 33, 421-431. 
Lois, J. (1999). Socialization to heroism: Individualism and collectivism in a voluntary search and rescue group. Social Psychology Quarterly, 62, 117-135.
London, P. (1970). The rescuers: Motivational hypotheses about Christians who saved Jews from the Nazis. In J. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social psychological studies of some antecedents and consequences (pp. 241-250). London: Academic Press.
Merrell, J. (2000). Ambiguity: Exploring the complexity of roles and boundaries when working with volunteers in well woman clinics. Social Science & Medicine, 51, 93-102.
Monnickendam, M., Monnickendam, S. M., Katz, C., & Katan, J. (2007). Health care for the poor – An exploration of primary care physicians’ perceptions of poor patients and of their helping behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 64, 1463-1474.
Monroe, K. R., Barton, M. C., & Klingemann, U. (1990). Altruism and the theory of rational action: rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. Ethics, 101, 103-122. 
Morgan, S. E. (2006). In their own words: The reasons why people will (not) sign an organ donor card. Health Communication, 23 (1), 23-33.
Mowen, J. C., & Sujan, H. (2005). Volunteer behavior: a hierarchical model approach for investigating its trait and functional motive antecedents. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15, 170-182. 
Nelson, J. A., & Folbre, N. (2006). Why a well-paid nurse is a better nurse. Nurse Economics, 24 (3), 127-130.
Newton, J. D. (2011). How does the general public view posthumous organ donation? A meta-synthesis of the qualitative literature. BMC Public Health, 11, 791.
Nilsson, J. (2009). Segmenting socially responsible mutual fund investors: The influence of financial return and social responsibility. International Journal of Bank Marketing27(1), 5-31.
Nimmons, D., & Folkman, S. (1999). Other-sensitive motivation for safer-sex among gay men: expanding paradigms for HIV prevention. Aids and Behavior, 3, 313-324.
O’Carroll, R.E., Dryden, J., Hamilton-Barclay, T., & Ferguson, E. (2011a). Anticipated regret and organ donor registration - A pilot study. Health Psychology, 30 (5), 661-664.
O’ Carroll, R. E., Foster, C., McGeechan, G., Sandford, K. & Ferguson, E. (2011b). The ``ick'' factor, anticipated regret, and willingness to become an organ donor. Health Psychology, 30 (2), 236-245. 
Omoto, A. M., & Snyder, M. (1995). Sustained helping without obligation: motivation, longevity of service, and perceived attitude change among AIDS volunteers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 671-686.  
Park, H. S. & Smith, S. W. (2007). Distinctiveness and influence of subjective norms, personal descriptive and injunctive norms, and societal descriptive and injunctive norms on behavioral intent: A case of two behaviors critical to organ donation. Human Communication Research, 33 (2), 194-218.
Passini, S., & Morselli, D. (2011). In the name of democracy: Disobedience and value-oriented citizenship. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 21 (3), 255-267.
Penner, L. A., & Finkelstein, M. A. (1998). Dispositional and structural determinants of volunteerism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74, 525-537. 
Piferi, R. L., Jobe, R. L., & Jones, W. H. (2006). Giving to others during national tragedy: The effects of altruistic and egoistic motivations on long-term giving. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 171-184. 
Rand, D. G., & Epstein, Z. G. (2014). Risking your life without a second thought: Intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. Forthcoming in PLoS ONE. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2424036
Reicher, S. Cassidy, C., Wolpert, I., & Hopkins, N., & Levine, M. (2006). Saving Bulgaria’s Jews: An analysis of social identity and the mobilisation of social solidarity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 49-72.
Robbins, M. L., Levesque, D. A., Redding, C. A., Johnson, J. L., & Prochaska, J. O. (2001). Assessing family members’ motivational readiness and decision making for consenting to cadaveric organ donation. Journal of Health Psychology¸6, 523-535.
Rotolo, T., & Wilson, J. (2006). Substitute or complement? Spousal influences on volunteering. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 305-319. 
Russell, G. M. (2011). Motives of heterosexual allies in collective action for equality. Journal of Social Issues, 67 (2), 376-393.
Russell, S. (1999). An exploratory study of patients' perceptions, memories and experiences of an intensive care unit. Journal of Advanced Nursing29(4), 783-791.
Schervish, P. G. (2005). Major donors, major motives: the people and purposes behind major gifts. New Directions for Philanthropic Fundraising, 47, 59-87.
Schroer, J., & Hertel, G. (2009). Voluntary engagement in an open web-based encyclopedia: Wikipedians and why they do it. Media Psychology12(1), 96-120.
Schultz, P. W., Gouveia, V. V., Cameron, L. D., Tankha, G., Schmuck, P., & Franŭk, M. (2005). Values and their relationship to environmental concern and conservation behaviour.  Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 457-475. 
Shepela, S. T., Cook, J., Horlitz, E., Leal, R., Luciano, S., Lufty, E., Miller, C., Mitchell, G., & Worden, E. (1999). Courageous resistance: a special case of altruism. Theory & Psychology, 9, 787-805.  
Shim, E., Chapman, G. B., Townsend, J. P., & Galvani, A. P. (2012). The influence of altruism on influenza vaccination decisions. Journal of The Royal Society Interface, 9(74), 2234-2243.
Siegel, J. T., Navarro, M. A., Tan, C. N., & Hyde, M. K. (2014). Attitude–behavior consistency, the principle of compatibility, and organ donation: A classic innovation. Health Psychology, 33(9), 1084-1091.
Snyder, M., Omoto, A. M., & Crain, A. L. (1999). Punished for their good deeds Stigmatization of AIDS volunteers. American Behavioral Scientist42(7), 1175-1192.
Sojka, B. N., & Sojka, P. (2008). The blood donation experience: Self-reported motives and obstacles for donating blood. Vox Sanguinis, 94 (1), 56–63.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Human Ecology Review, 6, 81-97.
Stukas, A. A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (1999). The effects of “mandatory volunteerism” on intentions to volunteer. Psychological Science10(1), 59-64.
Van der Cingel, M. (2011). Compassion in care: A qualitative study of older people with a chronic disease and nurses. Nursing Ethics, 18 (5), 672-685.
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., & Joiner Jr, T. E. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review117(2), 575.
van Zyl, L., & Walker, R. (2012). Beyond altruistic and commercial contract motherhood: The professional model. Bioethics, 27(7), 373-381.
Victoroff, J. (2005). The mind of a terrorist: A review and critique of psychological approaches. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 49, 3-42.
Wright, B. E. (2007). Public service and motivation: Does mission matter? Public Administration Review, 67 (1), 54-64.
Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C., Rozin, P., & Schwartz, B. (1997). Jobs, careers, and callings: people’s relations to their work. Journal of Research in Personality, 31 (1), 21-33.  


Picture credits

Scream [Link]
Taxis [Link]

How to cite this blog post using APA Style

T. Farsides. (2014, October 13). Real world altruism. Retrieved from http://tomfarsides.blogspot.com/2014/10/real-world-altruism.html  

2 comments:

  1. It is a very informative and useful post thanks it is good material to read this post increases my knowledge. Grass Valley Taxi

    ReplyDelete