Introductory waffle
This is very much a work-in-progress blog post, written in a bit of a rush. I hope to edit it ‘sometime’ to make it more clear, readable, and engaging, especially for non psychology students.
During the second seminar of my altruism module, each
student chooses a behaviour that might sometimes be, at least in part,
motivated by altruism, i.e., concern for the positive welfare of another or others. They can but do not have to choose
from the following list:
adoption, blood donation,
body-part (e.g., bone marrow, egg, sperm, organ) donation, business practices,
caring, citizenship, courageous resistance (including whistle-blowing),
courtesy, driving, environmentalism and animal welfare, heroism and emergency
intervention, honesty, internet behaviour, liberalism, lying, mercy killing,
philanthropy (charitable giving), religious action, rescuing, romantic
relationships, sexual behaviour, shopping, social activism, surrogacy,
tax-paying, terrorism, tipping, vaccination, volunteering, working, etc.
Each student then spends part of the following week
researching links between altruism (as defined above) and their chosen
behaviour. They produce a poster summarising what they have found important or
interesting. In the third seminar, they explain their posters to each other and
explore implications of similarities and differences across behaviours. We end
with a whole-group discussion. The vast majority of students put a lot of
thought and effort into their posters, which do not contribute to the grade
they will get for the module. Thanks, guys. You rock.
Below are some of the more important issues that tend to come
out during whole group discussions of students’ findings – albeit sometimes
with a heavy steer from me.
Behavioural specificity
If one’s goal is to identify and understand causes of something, it is crucially important to be very precise about what that thing is. Otherwise explaining, identifying, measuring, and manipulating that thing will at best be a hit-and-miss affair. At worst, the misses will far outnumber the hits.
I have spent a considerable amount of time detailing
precisely how I think the term “altruism” can usefully be understood (e.g., here,
here,
here
and especially here).
It is important to also be as clear as possible about any behaviour alleged to stem
from or express such altruism.
“Organ donation”, for example, is not the same thing as
“giving consent for one’s organs to be posthumously removed and used for
transplant or research purposes”. It is common for people to be enthusiastic
about the former general concept but disconcerted when thinking about the
latter specific possibility. As recognised long ago (Farsides, 2000) and
recently demonstrated empirically (Seiegal et al., 2014), attitudes towards these
distinct things can differ considerably.
This is not pure semantics. Champions of behavioural markers
of altruism rarely give due attention to what, specifically, they mean by the
target behaviour. This is particularly true if common terminology sneaks in
words that might encourage assumptions of an altruistic motive, e.g., “caring”,
“donating”, “giving”, “volunteering”, etc.. It is perfectly possible to “give”
someone a lift in a car in return for money - in which case, of course, it is
not a gift.
“Sure, I can give you a lift”
Which of the following is the behaviour that best represents
a specific act of “blood donation”: allowing someone to take blood out of one’s
arm; allowing someone to take a blood sample to be tested for a medical
condition; allowing someone to take blood from one’s arm in return for pay; allowing
a vampire to feed on one’s blood in exchange for a promise of immortality; or
allowing someone to take blood from one’s arm because one has an expectation that it will be used to try to
improve someone else’s welfare? If one chooses the first option it encompasses
all the others and there will be no distinction between people ‘giving’ blood
for personal gain and people giving blood in an attempt to help others (or
indeed for any other reason). If one wishes to restrict the phrase “personal
act of organ donation” to something like the final option (as I would), one no
longer has a strictly behavioural indicator. Instead, one needs to have a
pretty good idea that people are giving
or donating their blood in an attempt
to improve others’ welfare. One needs to know something about their goals and
motives.
Monroe et al. (1990) and London (1970) reached very
different conclusions about the motives of people who engaged in the behaviour
of “rescuing Jews from Nazi persecution in wartime Europe”. This may be because
London allowed in his sample people who received pay for their rescue and
Monroe et al. omitted such people. “Rescuing”, “rescuing for pay”, and
“rescuing without pay” are arguably very different behaviours, quite possibly
with distinct antecedents, explanations, and consequences.
Ditto “volunteering”, “mandatory volunteering”, and
voluntary volunteering (Stukas et al., 1999).
Now you try! What is an act of “giving to charity?” Is it
transferring ownership of money or other resources from one person to a charity
(for whatever reason, e.g., tax-avoidance); is it a person doing this
specifically in an attempt to help the charity in some way (e.g., as a vanity
project); or is it a person doing this in an attempt to help whoever or
whatever the charity claims to act in support of? These are not identical things! Only the
first is purely a behaviour. The others are behaviours engaged in for
particular reasons.
Focused altruism
When they are altruistic, people want good things for specific others. Their attitude towards people and things other than the targets of altruism (including their selves) cannot be taken for granted: it could be similarly positively concerned, indifferent, or indeed aggressive. Someone who wants a particular charity to thrive may not care about the fate of other charities. Someone who wants a particular country to thrive may want other countries to suffer in pursuit of this goal. People who want the best possible life for their children may have no active concern about any other child.
Multiple motives
People can have different motives for doing the same behaviour. Any behavior that is sometimes motivated at least in part by concern for the positive welfare of others is sometimes also or instead motivated by non-altruistic concerns. I cannot think of a single exception to this claim.
This is true for any all ‘prosocial’ or ‘helping’ behaviours,
including caring (Cicirelli, 1993; Feeney
& Collins, 2003); celebrity endorsement (Kelly et al., 2014); community
involvement (Batson et al., 2002); egg donation (Kenney & McGowan, 2008; Limiñana-Gras et al., 2014);
environmentalism (Gagnon-Thompson & Barton, 1994 ; Schultz et al., 2005); giving
blood (Sojka & Sojka, 2008); ethical purchasing and investment (Bravo et
al., 2012; Nilsson, 2009); organ donation (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003; Guedj
et al., 2011; Robbins et al., 2001; Sanner, 2006); philanthropy (Piferi et al., 2006; Schervish,
2005); rescuing (Reicher et al., 2006); safer sex (Nimmons & Folkman, 1999);
social activism (Cable et al, 1988; Russell, 2011); surrogacy (Blyth, 1994; van Zyl & Walker, 2012);
and volunteering (Allison et al., 2002; Clary et al., 1998; Omoto & Snyder,
1995).
It is also true that practices often engaged in for
self-interested reasons can also or instead be pursued for altruistic ones, for
example paid work (Amabile et al., 1994; Brewer et al., 2002; Coursey &
Pandey, 2007; Wrzesniewski et al.,
1997), vaccination (Hershey et al., 1994), staying alive
(Russell, 1999), and suicide (Blake, 1978; Van Orden
et al., 2010).
Similarly, behaviours many think of as immoral, antisocial,
or otherwise inappropriate can also be motivated, at least sometimes and in
part, by concern for the positive welfare of others, e.g., cheating (Gino et
al., 2013); civil disobedience (Passini & Morselli, 2011); lying (DePaulo
& Kashy, 1998); killing and letting die (Freidman & Resnick, 2007; Georges et al., 2007); military
disobedience (Linn, 1996); terrorism (Ginges et al., 2011; Kruglanski &
Fishman, 2009; Victoroff, 2005); and whistle-blowing (Kesselheim et al.,
2010).
Finally, sometimes people refrain from particular behaviours because they think that any
attempted help would not actually be in the best interests of those whose
welfare they care about (Gilbert & Silvera, 1996).
A consequence of all this is that behavioural markers of
altruism are a lot more difficult to identify than might be thought. Many
behaviours that are often motivated by concern for the positive welfare of
others are sometimes motivated by narrow self-interest (or worse) and many
behaviours (including “doing nothing”) that are often motivated by narrow
self-interest (or worse) are sometimes motivated, at least in part, by concern
for the positive welfare of others.
To be scientific and to have a justified claim to inform
policy and practice, theories of altruism need to explain and be informed by
people’s behaviour. But a science of altruism is unlikely to produce good
theories unless careful attention is paid to the relationship between
particular behaviours and the role played in their production by concerns for
the welfare of others (and other motives). Many theories considered on this
module make bold claims to be scientific because they focus on behaviours and not the motives that may lead to them.
Such an approach may (or may not) provide good accounts of the origins of those
behaviours but they (a) need to be very clear about the defining
characteristics of those behaviours, and (b) should not assume that those theories tell us tell us very much
about the phenomenon I am most interested in, i.e., concern for the positive
welfare of others. This is even true - indeed it is especially true - if they
claim to be studying “altruistic” behaviours.
“X can cause Y” does not entail that “X (only and always) causes Y”
That “X can cause Y” does not mean either that “X will always cause Y” or that “X is the only cause of Y”. Milgram (1970) demonstrated that obedience to authority can cause people to administer apparently fatal electric shocks to someone pleading for their torture to stop. This does not mean that obedience to authority always leads to torture (e.g., an authority may forbid torture) and nor does it mean that people only torture others because of orders to do so (e.g., they may take it upon themselves to engage in torture whether authorities encourage or dissuade such behaviour).
I honestly find it difficult to believe how many ‘scientific’
papers demonstrate that a particular motive can lead to an allegedly altruistic
behaviour and then invite or reach the conclusion that the former “therefore”
(always) leads to the latter and that the latter “therefore” is always the
result of the former. This drives me nuts!
Let me give you an example; one of many.
Griskevicius et
al. (2010) demonstrated that people’s concerns for status can motivate
them to pay high prices for ‘green’ products when doing so is a route to the status
they desire. I have no issue with this other than thinking, “Well, duh!” I do
have an issue with a claim that such a finding provides good evidence either
that people with status concerns will always pay high prices for ‘green’
products or, more importantly, that all behaviour apparently motivated by
environmental or altruistic concerns is in fact always and only motivated by
status concerns. To me, such a conclusion is beyond silly. Readers doubtful about
this are invited to re-read the section immediately above.
Changing Motives
Reasons for initially engaging in particular behaviours may differ from reasons for continuing to engage in those behaviours. People may start doing something for one reason but keep doing it for other reasons. People may also start doing something for one reason and then stop doing it for exactly the same reason, e.g., if someone volunteers to cheer themselves up but then discovers that volunteering makes them more miserable.
Accordingly, people may start doing something for altruistic
reasons but continue for non-altruistic reasons (Nelson & Folbre, 2006) or
vice versa (Callero et al., 1987; Charng et al., 1988; Ferguson et al., 2012).
Similarly, people may start something for one set of reasons but stop for the
same or for different reasons (Beldad et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2003; Schroer
& Hertel, 2009; Snyder et al., 1999).
In other words, just as motives can differ across people
(see “Multiple motives” above), they can also change (‘within’ people) across
time.
Situational motives
Motives can also differ across situations. Concern for the positive welfare of others depends on all manner of things that vary across situations, e.g., the others’ perceived need, perceived opportunities for change, how much the others are considered deserving of good or bad things happening to them, etc. We will study a whole raft of such factors later in the module.
Mixed Motives
People often have a variety of motives that exist at the same time or in quick succession. Motives can act in concert to promote particular behaviours or they can be incompatible and result in confusion or apparent inconsistency (Campbell, 2010; Merrell, 2000).
The considerations in the last few sections reiterate the
main point made at the end of the first: there is no one-to-one correspondence
between a particular behaviour (e.g., giving money to someone else) and a
particular concern motivating that behaviour (e.g., altruism, narrow
self-interest, morality, aggression...). The same motive (or set of motives)
can lead to different behaviours and the same behaviour can stem from different
motives (or different sets of motives). As well as being important when trying
to predict and explain particular behaviours, such issues will be important
again next week when we consider measures of altruism.
It’s not just self vs. other
Because of the history of the word “altruism” and on-going theoretical confusion, it is common for people to consider altruism in terms of a conflict between self- and other- interest. It is important to realise that there are far more interests at stake than this.
Because people can be concerned about the positive welfare
of various others, altruistic
concerns can co-exist and sometimes conflict with each other and with other,
non-altruistic, concerns. People can be simultaneously concerned about “the
general good”, the welfare of various specific others, their own welfare, doing
what they consider to be God’s will, etc. (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2010; Kurtz et
al., 2005).
“If I had to choose between betraying my country and
betraying my friend I hope I should have the guts to betray my country”. E. M.
Forster
Multiple methods
People can pursue all manner of different methods to seek to establish particular aspects of others’ positive welfare (Monnickendam et al., 2007; Van der Cingel, 2011). Someone who “wants to make the world a better place” may attempt to do so by giving blood, being political active, volunteering, or doing any number of other things. This again reiterates the point that there is not a one-to-one correspondence between altruism and any particular behaviour.
Altruism matters
It is common to hear people say things like, “As long as the behaviour keeps happening, who cares what motivates it or if such motives are altruistic or not?” I do and I think you should.
If people help others only instrumentally, in service of,
for example, narrow self-interest, they are likely to provide such help only to
the extent that it keeps serving that particular interest. If those motives are
better served by not helping, then helping will stop. If the motives are
adequately served by low-effort, poor-quality behaviour, then low-effort, poor
quality helping is what is likely to be provided. If the motives come to be
seen as better served by being unhelpful or maybe even callous or aggressive,
well...
To the extent that people help others because they seek
those others’ positive welfare, their helping will track what seems to be in
the others’ interest and it will be less dictated and distracted by other
(e.g., self-) interests.
In lots of different ways, people provide better care for
others when they genuinely care about them (i.e., about their positive welfare) than when they provide ‘care’ primarily in
pursuit of narrow self-interest (i.e., to obtain benefits solely for the self):
Bendapudi et al. (1996);
Cappellari and Turati (2004); Clary and Miller (1986); Finkelstein (2009); Horn (2012); Mowen and
Sujan (2005); Penner and Finkelstein
(1998); Shim et al. (2012); van
Zyl and Walker (2012); Wright (2007).
Altruism can be rewarding
People concerned about the positive welfare of others can find helping them rewarding (Berg et al., 2001; Brouwer et al., 2005; Harbaugh et al., 2007). This does not mean that they are impervious to costs of helping (Snyder et al., 1999).
Enough is enough
People hope for satisfaction of their desires and seek satisfaction of their motives. People’s desires and motives fade when satisfied. People who merely want to make a donation to charity want to do that. They do not necessarily want to give to the most effective charity and they will not necessarily welcome an ‘opportunity’ to work out which of several charities is most effective (Breeze, 2010; Buchheit & Parsons, 2006).
Any reader doubtful about this might want to re-read the “Behavioural
specificity” section above.
Barriers can be as important as motives
Encouraging altruism has two often-related but clearly distinct aspects: encouraging particular motives and removing barriers that inhibit people from having or acting on such motives.
Just as there are multiple motives for engaging in any
particular behaviour, so too are there multiple barriers (Guedj et al., 2011;
Hyde et al., 2012; Morgan, 2006; Newton, 2011; Sanner, 2006; Sojka & Sojka,
2008).
Context matters (a lot)
Altruism and helping are more pronounced in contexts where such qualities are valued and rewarded than they are in other contexts and they are least pronounced in contexts where those qualities are anathema and punished (Abolafia, 2002; Lois, 2007; Park & Smith, 2007; Rotolo & Wilson, 2006).
Ability and opportunity matters
Effective helping requires means and opportunities as well as motives (Bekkers, 2005; Gillath et al., 2005; Gross, 1994; Stern et al., 1999).
To the extent that altruistic helping requires specific
abilities and opportunities, distinct sets of qualities will need to accompany
altruism for such helping to be effectively provided (Rand & Epstein, 2014;
Shepela et al., 1999).
References
Abolafia, M.
Y. (2002). Making markets: Opportunism and restraint on Wall Street. In N. W.
Biggart (Ed.), Economic Sociology (Ch 6, pp. 94-111). Blackwell.
Allison, L.D;
Okun, M.A and Dutridge. K.S. (2002). Assessing volunteer motives: a comparison
of an open ended probe and Likert rating scales. Journal of Community and Applied Social
Psychology, 12, 243-255.
Amabile,
T.M., Hill, K.., Hennessey, B, A., & Tighe, E.M. (1994). The Work
Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational
orientations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 66, 950-967.
Batson,C.D.,
Ahmad,N., & Tsang, J-A. (2002). Four motives for community involvement. Journal
of Social Issues, 3, 429-446.
Bekkers, R.
(2005). Participation in voluntary associations: Relations with resources,
personality, and political values. Political
Psychology, 26 (3), 439-454.
Beldad, A.,
Snip, B., & van Hoof, J. (2012). Generosity the second time around:
Determinants of individuals’ repeat donation intention. Nonprofit and
Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 43(1), 144-163.
Bendapudi, N,
Singh, S. N., & Bendapudi, V. (1996). Enhancing helping behaviour: an
integrative framework for promotion planning. Journal of Marketing, 60,
33-49.
Berg, M. B., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Cotter,
J. (2001). Perceiving value in obligations and goals: Wanting to do what should
be done. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(8), 982-995. 9).
Blake, J. A.
(1978). Death by hand grenade: Altruistic suicide in combat. Suicide and
Life-Threatening Behavior, 8(1), 46-59.
Bleske-Rechek,
A., Nelson, L. A., Baker, J. P., Remiker, M. W., & Brandt, S. J. (2010).
Evolution and the trolley problem: People save five over one unless the one is
young, genetically related, or a romantic partner. Journal of Social,
Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 4(3), 115-127.
Blyth, E. (1994). “i wanted
to be interesting. i wanted to be able to say ‘i've done something interesting
with my life’”: Interviews with surrogate mothers in Britain. Journal of Reproductive
and Infant Psychology, 12(3), 189-198.
Bravo, C. P., Cordts, A., Schulze, B., & Spiller, A. (2012).
Assessing determinants of organic food consumption using data from the German
National Nutrition Survey II. Food Quality and Preference, 28
(1), 60-70.
Breeze, B.
(2010). How donors choose charities. Findings of a study of donor perceptions
of the nature and distribution of charitable benefit. Centre for Charitable
Giving and Philanthropy (CGAP), University of Kent. Ocassional Paper 1.
Brewer, G. A., Sheldon, S. C., & Facer, R. L. II. (2002). Individual
conceptions of public service motivation. Public
Administration Review, 60 (3),
254-264.
Brouwer, W. B. F., van Exel, N. J. A., van
der Berg, B., van den Bos, G. A. M. & Koopmanschap, M. A. (2005). Process
utility from providing informal care: The benefits of caring. Health Policy, 74, 85-99.
Buchheit, S., & Parsons, L. M. (2006). An experimental investigation of accounting information’s
influence on the individual giving process. Journal of Accounting and
Public Policy, 25(6), 666-686.
Cable, S.,
Walsh, E. J., & Warland, R. H. (1988). Differential paths to political
activism: Comparisons of four mobilization processes after the Three Mile
Island accident. Social Forces, 66, 951-969.
Callero, P. L., Howard, J. A., & Piliavin, J. A. (1987). Helping
behavior as role behavior: disclosing social structure and history in the
analysis of prosocial action. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50,
247-256.
Campbell, B.
(2010). Contradictory behaviour during genocides. Sociological Forum, 25
(2), 296-314.
Cappellari, L., & Turati,
G. (2004). Volunteer labour supply: The role of workers’ motivations. Annals
of Public and Cooperative Economics, 75(4), 619-643.
Charng, H-W.,
Piliavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role identity and reasoned action
in the prediction of repeated behaviour. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 51,
303-317.
Cicirelli, V.
G. (1993). Attachment and obligation as daughters’ motives for caregiving
behavior and subsequent effect on subjective burden. Psychology and Aging,
8, 144-155.
Clary, E. G.,
& Miller, J. (1986). Socialization and situational influences on sustained
altruism. Child Development, 57, 1358-1369.
Clary, E. G.,
Snyder, M., Ridge, R. D., Copeland, J., Stukas, A. A., Haugen, J., & Miene,
P. (1998). Understanding and assessing the motivations of volunteers: A functional
approach. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74,
1516-1530.
Coursey,
D. H., & Pandey, S. K. (2007). Public service motivation measurement: Testing an abridged version of Perry’s
proposed Scale.. Administration
and Society, 39 (5), 547-568.
Davis, M. E., Hall, J. A., & Meyer, M. (2003). The first year: Influences
on the satisfaction, involvement, and persistence of new community volunteers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
29, 248-260.
DePaulo,
B.M., & Kashy, D.A. (1998). Everyday lies in close and casual
relationships. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74,
63-79.
Farsides,
T. (2000). Winning hearts and minds: Using psychology to promote voluntary
organ donation. Health Care Analysis, 8, 101-121.
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2003).
Motivations for caregiving in adult intimate relationships: influences on
caregiving behaviour and relationship functioning. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 950-968.
Ferguson, E., Atsma, F., de Kort, W., & Veldhuizen, I. (2012).
Exploring the pattern of blood donor beliefs in first-time, novice, and
experienced donors: Differentiating reluctant altruism, pure altruism, impure
altruism, and warm glow. Transfusion, 52 (2), 343-355.
Finkelstein, M. A. (2009). Intrinsic vs.
extrinsic motivational orientations and the volunteer process. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 653-658.
Freidman, S. H., & Resnick, P. J. (2007).
Child murder by mothers: Patterns and prevention. World Psychiatry, 6 (3),
137 – 141.
Gagnon-Thompson,
S. C., & Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric
and anthropcentric attitudes towards the environment. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 14, 149-157.
Georges, J. J., Onwuteaka-Philipsen, B. D., Muller, M. T., Van der Wal, G.,
Van der Heide, A., & Van der Maas, P. J. (2007). Relatives' perspective on the
terminally ill patients who died after euthanasia or physician-assisted
suicide: a retrospective cross-sectional interview study in the Netherlands. Death
Studies, 31(1), 1-15.
Gilbert, D.
T., & Silvera, D. H. (1996). Overhelping. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 70, 678-690.
Gillath,
O., Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Nitzberg, R. E., Erez, A., & van
Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Attachment, caregiving, and volunteering: Placing
volunteerism in an attachment-theoretical framework. Personal Relationships, 12,
425-446.
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Sachdeva, S., &
Medin, D. (2011). Psychology out of the laboratory: The challenge of violent
extremism. American Psychologist, 66(6), 507.
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely,
D. (2013). Self-serving altruism? The lure of unethical actions that benefit
others. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93,
285-292.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van
den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and
conspicuous conservation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 98
(3), 392-404.
Gross, M. L.
(1994). Jewish rescue in Holland and France during the second world war: Moral
cognition and collective action. Social
Forces, 73, 463-496.
Guedj,
M., Sastre, M., & Mullet, E. (2011). Donating organs: A theory-driven
inventory of motives. Psychology, Health
& Medicine, 16 (4), 418-429.
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D.
R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for
charitable donations. Science, 316, 1622-1625.
Hershey, J. C., Asch, D. A.,
Thumasathit, T., Meszaros, J., & Waters, V. V. (1994). The roles of
altruism, free riding, and bandwagoning in vaccination decisions. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision
Processes, 59, 177–187.
Horn, A. S.
(2012). The cultivation of a prosocial value orientation through community
service: An examination of organizational context, social facilitation, and
duration. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 41 (7), 948-968.
Hyde, M. K.,
Wihardjo, K. R., & White, K. M. (2012). Do the myths still exist?
Revisiting people’s negative beliefs about organ donation upon death. Psychology,
Health & Medicine, 17(5), 530-541.
Johnson,
E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science, 302, 1338-1339.
Joiner, T.
E., Pettit, J. W., Walker, R. L., Voelz, Z. R., Cruz, J., Rudd, M. D., &
Lester, D. (2002). Perceived burdensomeness and suicidality: Two studies on the
suicide notes of those attempting and those completing suicide. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21(5), 531-545.
Kelly, S. P., Morgan, G. G.,
&Coule, T. M. (2014). Celebrity altruism: The good, the bad and the ugly in
relationships with fundraising charities. International Journal of
Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 19(2), 57-75.
Kenney, N.
J., & McGowan, M. L. (2008). Looking back: Egg donors’ retrospective
evaluations of their motivations, expectations, and experiences during their
first donation cycle. Fertility and
Sterility, 93(2), 455-466.
Kesselheim, A. S.,
Studdert, D. M., Mello, M. M. (2010). Whistle-blowers'
experiences in fraud litigation against pharmaceutical companies. New
England Journal of Medicine, 362, 1832-1939.
Kruglanski, A.
W., & Fishman, S. (2009). Psychological
factors in terrorism and counterterrorism: individual, group and organisational
levelsof analysis. Social Issues and
Policy Review, 3 (1), 1-44.
Kurtz, T.,
Donaghue, N., Rapley, M., & Walker, I. (2005). The ways that people talk
about natural resources: Discursive strategies as barriers to environmentally
sustainable practices. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44,
603-620.
Limiñana-Gras, R. M.,
Colodro-Conde, L., Andrés-Giménez, E. D., Sarabia-Cos, L., Vincenti, M. A.,
Giral-Sánchez, B., & Ruíz-Mira, C. M. (2014). Altruism and oocyte donation:
Motivational dimensions and decisions in women donors. Personality and
Individual Differences, 60, S50.
Linn, R.
(1996). When the individual soldier says “no” to war: A look at selective
refusal during the Intifada. Journal of
Peace Research, 33, 421-431.
Lois,
J. (1999). Socialization to heroism: Individualism and collectivism in a
voluntary search and rescue group. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 62,
117-135.
London, P.
(1970). The rescuers: Motivational hypotheses about Christians who saved Jews
from the Nazis. In J. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and
helping behavior: Social psychological studies of some antecedents and
consequences (pp. 241-250). London: Academic Press.
Merrell, J. (2000). Ambiguity: Exploring
the complexity of roles and boundaries when working with volunteers in well
woman clinics. Social Science & Medicine, 51, 93-102.
Monnickendam, M., Monnickendam, S. M., Katz, C.,
& Katan, J. (2007). Health care for the poor – An exploration of primary
care physicians’ perceptions of poor patients and of their helping behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 64, 1463-1474.
Monroe, K. R., Barton, M. C., &
Klingemann, U. (1990). Altruism and the theory of rational action:
rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. Ethics, 101, 103-122.
Morgan, S. E. (2006). In their own words: The reasons why people
will (not) sign an organ donor card. Health
Communication, 23 (1), 23-33.
Mowen,
J. C., & Sujan, H. (2005). Volunteer
behavior: a hierarchical model approach for investigating its trait and
functional motive antecedents. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15,
170-182.
Nelson, J. A., & Folbre, N. (2006). Why
a well-paid nurse is a better nurse. Nurse
Economics, 24 (3), 127-130.
Newton, J. D.
(2011). How does the general public view posthumous organ donation? A
meta-synthesis of the qualitative literature. BMC Public Health, 11,
791.
Nilsson, J.
(2009). Segmenting socially responsible mutual fund investors: The influence of
financial return and social responsibility. International Journal of
Bank Marketing, 27(1), 5-31.
Nimmons, D.,
& Folkman, S. (1999). Other-sensitive motivation for safer-sex among gay
men: expanding paradigms for HIV prevention. Aids and Behavior, 3,
313-324.
O’Carroll,
R.E., Dryden, J., Hamilton-Barclay, T., & Ferguson, E. (2011a). Anticipated regret and organ donor
registration - A pilot study. Health
Psychology, 30 (5), 661-664.
O’ Carroll, R.
E., Foster, C., McGeechan, G., Sandford, K. & Ferguson, E. (2011b). The ``ick'' factor, anticipated regret,
and willingness to become an organ donor. Health
Psychology, 30 (2), 236-245.
Omoto,
A. M., & Snyder, M. (1995). Sustained helping without obligation:
motivation, longevity of service, and perceived attitude change among AIDS
volunteers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
671-686.
Park, H. S.
& Smith, S. W. (2007). Distinctiveness and influence of subjective norms,
personal descriptive and injunctive norms, and societal descriptive and
injunctive norms on behavioral intent: A case of two behaviors critical to
organ donation. Human Communication
Research, 33 (2), 194-218.
Passini, S.,
& Morselli, D. (2011). In the name of democracy: Disobedience and
value-oriented citizenship. Journal of Community and Applied Social
Psychology, 21 (3), 255-267.
Penner, L. A., & Finkelstein, M. A.
(1998). Dispositional and structural determinants of volunteerism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74, 525-537.
Piferi,
R. L., Jobe, R. L., & Jones, W. H. (2006). Giving to others during national tragedy: The effects of altruistic and
egoistic motivations on long-term giving. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 23,
171-184.
Rand, D. G.,
& Epstein, Z. G. (2014). Risking your life without a second thought:
Intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. Forthcoming in PLoS ONE.
Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2424036
Reicher, S.
Cassidy, C., Wolpert, I., & Hopkins, N., & Levine, M. (2006). Saving
Bulgaria’s Jews: An analysis of social identity and the mobilisation of social
solidarity. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 36, 49-72.
Robbins,
M. L., Levesque, D. A., Redding, C. A., Johnson, J. L., & Prochaska, J. O.
(2001). Assessing family members’ motivational readiness and decision making
for consenting to cadaveric organ donation. Journal of Health Psychology¸6,
523-535.
Rotolo,
T., & Wilson, J. (2006). Substitute or complement? Spousal influences on
volunteering. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 305-319.
Russell, G. M. (2011). Motives of
heterosexual allies in collective action for equality. Journal of Social
Issues, 67 (2), 376-393.
Russell, S.
(1999). An exploratory study of patients' perceptions, memories and experiences
of an intensive care unit. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 29(4),
783-791.
Schervish, P.
G. (2005). Major donors, major motives: the people and purposes behind major
gifts. New Directions for Philanthropic
Fundraising, 47, 59-87.
Schroer, J., & Hertel, G. (2009). Voluntary engagement in an open
web-based encyclopedia: Wikipedians and why they do it. Media
Psychology, 12(1), 96-120.
Schultz, P.
W., Gouveia, V. V., Cameron, L. D., Tankha, G., Schmuck, P., & Franŭk, M. (2005). Values
and their relationship to environmental concern and conservation behaviour. Journal
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36,
457-475.
Shepela, S. T., Cook, J.,
Horlitz, E., Leal, R., Luciano, S., Lufty, E., Miller, C., Mitchell, G., &
Worden, E. (1999). Courageous resistance: a special case of altruism. Theory
& Psychology, 9, 787-805.
Shim, E., Chapman, G. B.,
Townsend, J. P., & Galvani, A. P. (2012). The influence of altruism on
influenza vaccination decisions. Journal of The Royal Society Interface,
9(74), 2234-2243.
Siegel,
J. T., Navarro, M. A., Tan, C. N., & Hyde, M. K. (2014). Attitude–behavior
consistency, the principle of compatibility, and organ donation: A classic
innovation. Health Psychology, 33(9), 1084-1091.
Snyder, M.,
Omoto, A. M., & Crain, A. L. (1999). Punished for their good deeds
Stigmatization of AIDS volunteers. American Behavioral Scientist, 42(7),
1175-1192.
Sojka, B. N.,
& Sojka, P. (2008). The blood donation experience: Self-reported motives
and obstacles for donating blood. Vox
Sanguinis, 94 (1), 56–63.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano,
G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social
movements: The case of environmentalism. Human
Ecology Review, 6, 81-97.
Stukas, A.
A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (1999). The effects of “mandatory
volunteerism” on intentions to volunteer. Psychological Science, 10(1),
59-64.
Van der
Cingel, M. (2011). Compassion in care: A qualitative study of older people with
a chronic disease and nurses. Nursing Ethics, 18 (5), 672-685.
Van Orden, K.
A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., &
Joiner Jr, T. E. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological
Review, 117(2), 575.
van Zyl, L., & Walker, R.
(2012). Beyond altruistic and commercial contract motherhood: The professional
model. Bioethics, 27(7), 373-381.
Victoroff, J.
(2005). The mind of a terrorist: A review and critique of psychological
approaches. Journal of Conflict
Resolution, 49, 3-42.
Wright, B. E.
(2007). Public service and motivation: Does mission matter? Public Administration Review, 67 (1), 54-64.
Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C., Rozin, P.,
& Schwartz, B. (1997). Jobs, careers, and callings: people’s
relations to their work. Journal of
Research in Personality, 31 (1),
21-33.
Picture credits
Scream [Link]
Taxis [Link]
How to cite this
blog post using APA Style
T.
Farsides. (2014, October 13). Real world altruism. Retrieved from http://tomfarsides.blogspot.com/2014/10/real-world-altruism.html
It is a very informative and useful post thanks it is good material to read this post increases my knowledge. Grass Valley Taxi
ReplyDeleteI am so grateful for your blog.Really thank you!
ReplyDeleteGrass Valley Taxi to Airport